105x Filetype PDF File size 0.44 MB Source: www.psi.uba.ar
Psychotherapy Research ISSN: 1050-3307 (Print) 1468-4381 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tpsr20 Goldilocks on the couch: Moderate levels of psychodynamic and process-experiential technique predict outcome in psychodynamic therapy Kevin S. McCarthy, John R. Keefe & Jacques P. Barber To cite this article: Kevin S. McCarthy, John R. Keefe & Jacques P. Barber (2016) Goldilocks on the couch: Moderate levels of psychodynamic and process-experiential technique predict outcome in psychodynamic therapy, Psychotherapy Research, 26:3, 307-317, DOI: 10.1080/10503307.2014.973921 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2014.973921 Published online: 03 Nov 2014. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 276 View related articles View Crossmark data Citing articles: 1 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=tpsr20 Download by: [University of Pennsylvania] Date: 23 May 2017, At: 09:34 Psychotherapy Research, 2016 Vol. 26, No. 3, 307–317, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2014.973921 EMPIRICALPAPER Goldilocks on the couch: Moderate levels of psychodynamic and process-experiential technique predict outcome in psychodynamic therapy 1,2 2 3 KEVINS. MCCARTHY ,JOHNR.KEEFE ,&JACQUESP.BARBER 1Department of Psychology, Chestnut Hill College, Philadelphia, PA, USA; 2Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA & 3Derner Institute, Adelphi University, Garden City, NY, USA (Received 8 November 2013; revised 5 August 2014; accepted 29 September 2014) Abstract Objectives: Greater symptom change is often assumed to follow greater technique use, a “more is better” approach. We tested whether psychodynamic techniques, as well as common factors and techniques from other orientations, had a curvilinear relation to outcome (i.e., whether moderate or “just right” intervention levels predict better outcome than lower or higher levels). Methods: For 33 patients receiving supportive-expressive psychodynamic psychotherapy for depression, interventions were assessed at Week 4 using the multitheoretical list of therapeutic interventions and symptoms were rated with the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression. Results: Moderate psychodynamic and experiential techniques predicted greater symptom change compared to lower or higher levels. Conclusion: This “Goldilocks effect” suggests a more complex relation of intervention use to outcome might exist. Keywords: psychoanalytic/psychodynamic therapy; experiential/existential/humanistic psychotherapy; outcome research Modern psychodynamic therapy incorporates mul- outcome (cf., Stiles, 1996; Stiles, Honos-Webb, & tiple theories from its 120-year history to help Surko, 1998; Stiles & Shapiro, 1989, 1994)ora patients change (Mitchell & Black, 1996; Summers “more is better” approach (Barber, 2009; Stiles & &Barber, 2009). Even with this diversity of thought, Shapiro, 1989). For instance, quantitative investiga- most psychodynamic thinkers and practitioners tions of the relation of techniques to outcome have agree on several core techniques that constitute the almost exclusively relied on the use of linear correla- work of psychodynamic therapy (Blagys & Hilsen- tion models (for reviews, see Stiles & Shapiro, 1989; roth, 2000; Summers & Barber, 2009). Among Webb, DeRubeis, & Barber, 2010). them, supportive interventions reinforce adaptive However, the empirical association of dynamic responses (ego defenses) in the patient. These interventions to symptom change remains unclear interventions include relationship building, sugges- (for reviews, see Barber, Muran, McCarthy, & tion or advice giving, and boundary setting. Express- Keefe, 2013; Høglend, 2004). By and large, investi- ive techniques explore and uncover (express) the gations of global measures of psychodynamic inter- unconscious conflict behind the patient’s symptoms ventions and symptom change have been equivocal (Luborsky, 1984). These interventions include (for no relation, see Barber, Crits-Christoph, & exploration of affect and interpersonal themes and Luborsky, 1996; DeFife, Hilsenroth, & Gold, 2008; making connections between different relationships Ogrodniczuk & Piper, 1999; Ogrodniczuk, Piper, in the patient’s life (transference interpretations). Joyce, & McCallum, 2000; for a favorable relation, Most often it is assumed that greater use of these see Ablon & Jones, 1998; Gaston, Thompson, techniques is likely to lead to improved patient Gallagher, Cournoyer, & Gagnon, 1998; Hendriksen Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kevin S. McCarthy, Department of Psychology, Chestnut Hill College, Philadelphia, PA, USA. Email: kevin.mccarthy@chc.edu ©2014 Society for Psychotherapy Research 308 K. S. McCarthy et al. et al., 2011; Hilsenroth, Ackerman, Blagys, Baity, meta-analysis that varied from strongly negative to & Mooney, 2003; Luborsky, McLellan, Woody, strongly positive, which lends partial support to a O’Brien, & Auerbach, 1985; for an unfavorable curvilinear hypothesis. relation, see Barber et al., 2008). In the only meta- It is interesting and perhaps surprising that only a analysis to date on the subject, Webb et al. (2010) few studies have tested a curvilinear relation of found no significant relation between adherence dynamic interventions and symptom change. In the (i.e., the degree to which therapists deliver theory- first empirical study of hypothesis, only partial specific interventions which are consistent with a support for a curvilinear relation between interpreta- therapy manual) and symptomatic improvement tion and outcome was found (Piper et al., 1991). across 32 studies of psychotherapy outcome, regard- Two later correlational studies did not produce less of the therapeutic modality under study (e.g., evidence that moderate dynamic technique use was dynamic, interpersonal, cognitive-behavioral, and associated with better outcome than lower or higher process-experiential). levels of technique use (Barber et al., 2008; Ogrod- While the disappointing link between technique niczuk & Piper, 1999). Indeed, the opposite effect use and outcome is not unique to psychodynamic was observed for a sample of patients in dynamic therapy alone (Stiles, 1996; Wampold, 2001; Webb therapy for cocaine dependence (very high and very et al., 2010), it raises the question about how low level of dynamic interventions were related to techniques might have their effect in therapy. Some better outcome than were moderate levels; Barber 1 researchers have productively explored the interac- et al., 2008). A unique experimental study tested tion of dynamic techniques and therapeutic alliance whether patients with mixed diagnoses improved in predicting outcome (e.g., Barber et al., 2008; more when randomly assigned to psychodynamic Gaston et al., 1998; Høglend et al., 2011; Owen & treatment with a moderate level of transference Hilsenroth, 2011). Others have examined how com- interpretations (1–3 per session) or a low level of petent delivery of dynamic techniques relates to interpretations (Høglend et al., 2006). Patients symptomchange(e.g., Barber et al., 1996). Notably, receiving both low and moderate levels of interpreta- Stiles and colleagues (1989, 1994, 1996, 1998) have tion improved significantly over time, but there was cautioned against the expectation of a simple cor- little difference in the amount that patients in each relation between process and outcome factors and condition changed (although moderating effects of have suggested the responsiveness of the therapist to patient characteristics were later found, Høglend the patient’s current needs is what might facilitate et al., 2011). The researchers chose not to include change. This hypothesis agrees with the experience a high interpretation level condition in their design of many dynamic practitioners. The psychodynamic because their review of naturalistic studies of psycho- clinical literature has long suggested that too many dynamic process and outcome suggested that such a interpretative interventions may be disruptive to the condition was unlikely to be effective in terms of patient’s functioning (Strachey, 1934; Gill, 1982). outcome and cost (Per Høglend, personal commun- Too many supportive interventions may overly grat- ication 02/15/10). A curvilinear relation may there- ify the patient and mask his or her symptoms without fore still exist between psychodynamic techniques bringing longer term relief or might trigger an and outcome in patients with internalizing disorders. enactment of his or her conflict in the therapeutic Delivery of any psychotherapy is also likely to relationship (Freud, 1919; Gill, 1951; Kohut & involve interventions from numerous schools of Wolf, 1978). treatment. For example, it has been demonstrated Accordingly, some researchers have hypothesized that psychodynamic therapy contains modest levels a curvilinear relation between psychodynamic inter- of techniques from other therapy systems, like drug ventions and outcome (Piper, Azim, Joyce, & counseling (Barber et al., 2008), cognitive therapy McCallum, 1991; see also Barber et al., 2008; (DeFife et al., 2008), and a number of different Høglend et al., 2006; Ogrodniczuk & Piper, 1999). other theoretical orientations (McCarthy & Barber, Namely, moderate levels of dynamic interventions 2009; Trijsburg et al., 2002). The effect of dynamic might be related to greater symptom improvement interventions on symptom improvement in other than might very high and very low levels (a “just types of therapies has been reviewed elsewhere (see right” hypothesis). The equivocal results of previous Shedler, 2010), but considerations of how techni- studies might have either represented a single leg of ques from other therapies might influence outcome the curvilinear relation (either the positive or negat- in dynamic therapy have been less systematic. In a ive association of techniques to symptom improve- handful of studies, unintended techniques have had ment) or might have represented a linear model a modest to substantial contribution to outcome in being forced on curvilinear data. Webb and collea- dynamic psychotherapy (Ablon & Jones, 1998; gues (2010) observed individual effect sizes in their Barber et al., 2008; DeFife et al., 2008; Luborsky Psychotherapy Research 309 et al., 1985; but see also Hilsenroth et al., 2003). Fifty-one patients were initially randomized to However, in each of these studies interventions from psychodynamic therapy. However, nine dropped only a few psychotherapy systems have been exam- out of treatment before the first process measure- ined (e.g., Luborsky et al., 1985), which under- ment point, five of whom never attended their first represents the variety of interventions from different therapy appointment. Of these nine patients, one systems that dynamic therapists might possibly use. described practical reasons for dropping out of A curvilinear relation of unintended techniques to treatment (e.g., childcare and transportation), two outcome might also exist as we hypothesize for stated that the time commitment was too great, three dynamic therapy techniques. Very high levels of wanted a treatment other than dynamic therapy, and interventions from any system might represent tech- three did not say why they exited the study or could nical rigidity or inflexibility to the patient’s needs on not be reached. Eight of the nine patients leaving the part of the therapist (Miller & Binder, 2002), treatment before Week 8 were of minority status whereas very low levels of interventions from any (i.e., African-American or Latino). An additional system may not be sufficient to motivate change in nine patients discontinued treatment after Week 4 or the patient. declined to participate in a termination interview. The present study tests the curvilinear relation of Onemovedawayfromthearea, one dropped out for a wide range of interventions from a number of practical reasons, two felt improved and did not theoretical orientations to subsequent outcome want further treatment, three felt they were not in psychodynamic therapy for depression. More improving fast enough, one was dissatisfied with specifically, we hypothesize that moderate levels of her treatment, and one did not say. Five of these psychodynamic interventions (e.g., exploration and nine patients leaving treatment before termination interpretation) will predict more symptom improve- were of minority status. The subsample of 33 ment than will higher or lower levels of dynamic completers did not differ significantly from the larger interventions. We also hypothesize that supportive sample on demographic characteristics, process interventions (e.g., common factors like providing measures, or outcome measures. hope and fostering therapeutic alliance) will be Sixty-one percent of patients were female (n = 20). associated with outcome, although we have no Mean age was 35.5 years (SD = 12.0, range = 19– strong predictions for whether this relation will be a 58). Six percent identified their primary race or ethnicity as Asian (n = 2), 49% as African-American linear or curvilinear function. We will also explore (n = 16), 3% as Latino/a (n = 1), and 42% as the contribution of interventions not intended to be Caucasian (n = 14). The majority of clients were found in dynamic therapy (i.e., behavioral, cognitive, single (n = 21, 64%), with fewer clients separated/ dialectical-behavioral, interpersonal, person-cen- divorced (n = 2, 6%) or married or cohabiting (n = tered, and process-experiential) to outcome. We do 9, 27%), and 1 (3%) widowed. Average number of not have specific directional hypotheses for the years of education completed was 14.3 (SD = 2.2, potential relations of interventions from these sys- range = 10–19). Fifty-five percent worked either full- tems to outcome, but will explore for their linear and (n = 14) or part-time (n = 4), 36% (n = 12) were curvilinear relation to outcome for interventions unemployed/disabled, and 9% (n = 3) were students. from these different systems separately. Therapists. Therapy was provided by four Ph.D.- level psychologists (three were female) with an Methods average of more than 15 years of psychotherapy Participants experience at the beginning of the trial. All therapists were between the ages of 40 and 50. All had received Patients. Participants were 33 patients complet- training in psychodynamic therapy prior to participa- ing treatment in the psychotherapy arm of a rando- tion in this study and had achieved acceptable levels mized controlled trial (RCT) comparing of adherence and competence using the Penn Adher- psychodynamic therapy versus pharmacotherapy ver- ence-Competence Scale (Barber & Critis-Christoph, sus pill placebo. To be included in the study 1996). The median number of clients that each participants were required to have a primary dia- therapist treated in this sample was eight. gnosis of major depressive disorder based on their responses to the structured clinical interview for Treatment DSM-IV(Axis I) administered by a trained diagnos- tician. They could not have lifetime history of The psychodynamic therapy conducted in this study bipolar or psychotic disorder nor a substance abuse followed a supportive-expressive (SE) treatment or dependence disorder in the previous 6 months. model (Luborsky, 1984) with specific adaptations
no reviews yet
Please Login to review.